The recent back-and-forth debate—and policy reversal—over the use of face masks to prevent the spread of Covid-19 reveals a glaring double standard. For some reason, we’ve been treating this one particular matter of public health differently. We don’t see op-eds that ask whether people really need to keep 6 feet away from each other on the street, as opposed to 3 feet, or that cast doubt on whether it’s such a good idea to promote bouts of handwashing that are 20 seconds long. But when it comes to covering our faces, a scholarly hyper-rigor has been applied. In recent weeks, experts have counseled caution—or rejected the use of masks by the general public outright—as they pleaded for better, more decisive evidence. Why?
They’re right, of course, that the research literature on mask usage doesn’t provide definitive answers. There are no large-scale clinical trials proving that personal use of masks can prevent pandemic spread; and the ones that look at masks and influenza have produced equivocal results. But this smattering of evidence doesn’t tell us much, either way: The trials neither prove that masks are useful, nor that they’re dangerous or a waste of time. That’s because the studies have been both few in number and beset with methodological problems.
Take, for example, a large randomized trial of mask use among US college students in the 2006–07 influenza season. The reduction in illness among those wearing face masks in that study was not statistically significant. But because the research was carried out during what turned out to be a mild season for the flu, the trial lacked statistical power for that question; there weren’t enough sick people for researchers to figure out whether wearing masks improved on hand hygiene alone. They also couldn’t rule out the possibility that students were already infected before the trial began.
Or take another study of the same influenza season, this time in Australia, which found no definitive effect. That one looked at adults living with children who had influenza. Less than half the people randomized into the group of mask wearers reported using them “most or all of the time.” In fact, they were often sleeping next to their sick children without them. This bears little resemblance to the question of whether you should wear a mask among strangers at the grocery store in the midst of a pandemic.
But here’s the thing: One could make the same complaints about the evidence supporting mask use by health care workers too. While everyone agrees this practice is absolutely critical in hospitals and clinics, that’s not because we have convincing proof from randomized trials. The few clinical trials we have of using masks for health care workers to prevent influenza don’t show a clear effect; nor can they even demonstrate that the more substantial N95 respirators work better than surgical masks. Those trials are also far from ideal. For example, one tested the efficacy of cloth masks by comparing health care workers who wore them to those wearing surgical masks or respirators, and also to a control group that followed “standard practice” in the hospital. It turned out that the majority of workers in the control group were wearing surgical masks anyway, so the study couldn’t really show whether the cloth masks were better (or worse) than wearing no masks at all.
Indeed, the scientific basis for health care workers using masks doesn’t come from clinical trials of influenza outbreaks or pandemics. It comes from laboratory simulations showing that masks can prevent viral particles from getting through—there are at least a couple dozen of those—and from case-control studies during the 2003 coronavirus epidemic that caused SARS. Those SARS studies weren’t limited to health care workers.
It’s true that health care workers or other people looking after people sick with Covid-19 are exposed to far higher levels of coronavirus than anyone else. In the context of a mask shortage, they obviously have priority claim to access. But that’s not a reason to say there isn’t support for the use of masks by everyone else. After all, there aren’t any clinical trials proving that a 6-foot social distance prevents infection, as far as we know. (The World Health Organization only recommends a 3-foot separation.) Nor do clinical trials prove that washing our hands for 20 seconds is superior to doing so for 10 seconds when it comes to limiting the spread of disease in a respiratory disease pandemic. The scientific basis for that 20-second handwashing advice from the US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention derives from laboratory studies measuring virus on the hands after different washing times.
So what was the source of this double standard regarding face masks—and why was it finally dropped?
I think it’s mostly because we have consistently underestimated this virus, while overestimating our own ability to deal with it. Miao Hua, an anthropologist and medical resident at Mount Sinai Hospital in New York City, was shocked by the difference in attitudes toward infection control in the US compared to Wuhan. In China, she wrote a few weeks ago, the spread inside hospitals quickly squelched the idea that routine containment strategies would be enough to stop this new coronavirus. What she was hearing from China was surreal, she said, and especially worrying in light of “the American medical community’s failure to register the historical uniqueness of Covid-19”.
The CDC’s recent policy change in support of masks suggests this long-overdue acknowledgement may finally have been made. The agency’s statement attributes the change to accumulating evidence that the disease isn’t transmitted in the same way as influenza: that people can be contagious and asymptomatic, and that the virus may be spread by talking, as well as coughing, sneezing, and contacting contaminated surfaces.
I think the reluctance to promote mask use by the general public, as well as the application of a double standard for supporting evidence, was also driven by concerns that people would be unable to use masks without contaminating themselves. Or that masks would provide a false sense of security, leading them to slacken off social distancing or other measures. Effective communication is key here, though, just as it has been for thorough handwashing technique. Stella Quah, a sociologist at the University of Singapore, studied the social aspects of the SARS epidemic in Singapore, where the public health campaign included education about hand hygiene, as well as taking temperatures and the proper use of face masks. The CDC reversed its face mask guidance last Friday, then posted some limited advice on how to wear and remove them, along with instructions for making your own from a combination of bandanas and coffee filters.
More education than that will be essential, though, if all those images we are seeing on TV of people with masks not covering their noses or chins are anything to go by. Recent history holds the same lesson. After Hurricane Katrina, respirators were recommended for anyone doing mold remediation work in New Orleans. A study of how that worked for a random sample of 538 residents showed the need for education: Only 24 percent were wearing them correctly, and they were often people who had used them before; meanwhile 22 percent of people put on their respirators upside down. The authors of that study concluded: “Interventions to improve respirator donning should be considered in planning or influenza epidemics and disasters.” A 2014 study in Wuhan found that proper use of respirators in non-health-care workers was quite a bit higher after training.
Could widespread (and proper) use of masks have made a difference where the virus escaped containment? A 2018 study by Jin Yan and colleagues from the US Food and Drug Administration constructed a model based on assumptions from laboratory data. They concluded that if only 20 percent of people use masks, it wouldn’t make a difference for the spread of influenza. At 50 percent compliance, though, with the use of high-filtration surgical masks, the effect might be substantial. That’s just a theoretical result, and we know that Covid-19 outbreaks have been contained in places without widespread use of masks. On the other hand, when an outbreak is out of control, even a small contribution matters.
In the end, it’s hard to escape the suspicion that the double standard about masks has less to do with science than a cultural difference over how we respond to pandemics. The difference has been evident since at least the first coronavirus pandemic, SARS, which changed attitudes and behaviors around public health in Asia. It’s not just about masks: Non-Asian countries have also behaved differently on screening people’s temperatures or disinfecting public spaces. There’s nothing new about this tendency, though. We often ask for extra-special proof when a practice doesn’t fit our preconceived ideas. That, unfortunately, is all too common; and scientists aren’t immune.
WIRED is providing free access to stories about public health and how to protect yourself during the coronavirus pandemic. Sign up for our Coronavirus Update newsletter for the latest updates, and subscribe to support our journalism.
WIRED is where tomorrow is realized. It is the essential source of information and ideas that make sense of a world in constant transformation. The WIRED conversation illuminates how technology is changing every aspect of our lives—from culture to business, science to design. The breakthroughs and innovations that we uncover lead to new ways of thinking, new connections, and new industries.
© 2020 Condé Nast. All rights reserved. Use of this site constitutes acceptance of our User Agreement (updated 1/1/20) and Privacy Policy and Cookie Statement (updated 1/1/20) and Your California Privacy Rights. Do Not Sell My Personal Information Wired may earn a portion of sales from products that are purchased through our site as part of our Affiliate Partnerships with retailers. The material on this site may not be reproduced, distributed, transmitted, cached or otherwise used, except with the prior written permission of Condé Nast. Ad Choices
Post time: Apr-09-2020